Session Information
09 SES 13 A, Analyzing Peer Group Effects
Paper Session
Contribution
Since the launch of the voucher system with free school choice in Sweden in the early 1990s, individual students were offered opportunities to attend schools of their own preferences. Even though a claimed intention of the free school choice was to reduce the educational inequality with respect to achievement differences and social background of students, empirical evidence has shown that the social background effect on academic achievement has been strengthened between different schools (Gustafsson & Yang Hansen, 2009).
One of the key factors behind the strengthened educational inequality is believed to be peer effects. Education policy changes, such as free school choice, often make substantial changes to the composition of a student's peer group, especially when such a policy is mostly taken advantage of by the well-educated middle class parents (Levin, 2002). It may be expected that the learning outcomes may be affected by the school/classroom mix. When students in schools or class are sorted by social, ethnic background and or prior achievement, both students’ cognitive and non-cognitive outcomes will be affected by the characteristics of their peers (Hanushek et al., 2003, Hoxby, 2000, Marsh et al., 2008).
Other school organizational factors, such as tracking and ability grouping also affect the composition of students in classrooms or schools. Informal ability grouping for instruction, however, has become more common in the recent years in Sweden (Mullis et al., 2012). Ammermüller & Pischke (2009) studied peer effects in six European countries using IES PIRLS 2001 data. Although the average peer effect estimated for Sweden was rather similar to those in the rest of the countries, they did find evidence that students in the Swedish sample may not have been randomly assigned to their classes.
Though research has demonstrated the importance of peer effects, the estimated size, however, was rather small. This is partly due to the methodological challenges in estimating peer effect, since the composition of students in schools and classrooms are very often not randomly formed. The endogeneity hence will bias the estimation of peer effects. One of the commonly used approaches is using the within-school variance to identify peer effect in a school fixed effect model. However, this approach assumes that the students in the classroom are randomly assigned because if student’s characteristics in the classrooms are related to the unmeasured components in the model, the estimation of variance between classrooms does not represent the peer characteristics.
However, Hoxby (2000) demonstrated that such selection biases can be tackled by analyzing adjacent cohorts simultaneously. She assumed that “there is some variation in the adjacent cohorts’ peer composition within a grade within a school that is idiosyncratic and beyond the easy management of parents and schools (p.3). However, such adjacent cohorts data is rarely available. The proposed study therefore is to explore the measurement of peer effects, and to estimate the size of peer effects in Sweden. Multiple methods will be tested and the results will be compared and interpreted.
Method
Expected Outcomes
References
Ammermüller, A. & Pischke, J-S. (2009). Peer Effects in European Primary Schools: Evidence from PIRLS. Journal of Labor Economics, 27(3), 315-348. Gustafsson, J.-E., & Yang Hansen, K. (2009). Resultatförändringar i svensk grundskola [Changes in Outcomes in Swedish Compulsory Schools; in Swedish]. In L. M. Olsson (Ed.), Vad påverkar resultaten i grundskolan? (pp. 40-84). Stockholm: Skolverket. Hanushek, E. A., Kain, J. F., Markman, J. M., & Rivkin, S. G. (2003). Does Peer Ability Affect Student Achievement? Journal of Applied Econometrics, 18(5), 527-544. Hoxby, C. (2000). Peer Effects in the Classroom: Learning from Gender and Race Variation. NBER working paper no. 7867. National Bureau of Economic Research. Hattie, J. (2009). Visible learning. A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. London: Routledge. Levin, H. M. (2002). A Comprehensive Framework for Evaluating Educational Vouchers. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 24, 159. Lüdtke, O., Marsh, H. W., Robitzsch, A., Trautwein, U., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2008). The multilevel latent covariate model: A new, more reliable approach to group-level effects in contextual studies Psychological Methods 13, 203-229. Marsh, H. W., Seaton, M., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Hau, K. T., O’Mara, A. J., & Craven, R. G. (2008). The big-fish-little-pond-effect stands up to critical scrutiny: Implications for theory, methodology, and future research. Educational Psychology Review, 20, 319-350. Mullis, I.V.S., Martin, M.O., Minnich, C.A., Drucker, K.T., & Ragan, M.A. (2012). PIRLS 2011 Encyclopedia: Education Policy and Curriculum in Readin.g Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College. Sahlberg, P. (2011). The Fourth Way of Finland. Journal of educational Changge, 12(2), 173-185. Sallis, J. F., Prochaska, J. J., & Taylor, W. C. (2000). A review of correlates of physical activity of children and adolescents. Medicine & Science in sports & Exercise, 32, 963-975. Sund, K. (2009). Estimating peer effects in Swedish high school using school, teacher, and student fixed effect. Economics of Education Review, 28(3), 329-336.
Search the ECER Programme
- Search for keywords and phrases in "Text Search"
- Restrict in which part of the abstracts to search in "Where to search"
- Search for authors and in the respective field.
- For planning your conference attendance you may want to use the conference app, which will be issued some weeks before the conference
- If you are a session chair, best look up your chairing duties in the conference system (Conftool) or the app.