Session Information
31 SES 11 B, Children's Language Development, Influencing Factors and Pedagogies
Paper Session
Contribution
Language skills are essential to successfully participate in education processes and in society more generally. In Germany between 15% and 50% of all children lack proficiency in the German language. Disadvantaged children (e.g. migrant children or children with a low socioeconomic status) are especially affected by this. Following socioecological and sociocultural theories on human development (e.g. Bronfenbrenner, 1990; Rogoff, 1990; Vygotski, 1978), the quality and quantity of the linguistic input these children experience can be considered as one of the reasons for their lack in language skills. These theories emphasize the importance of a meaningful and stimulating environment as well as social interactions for (language) development.
The family is a child’s first and foremost learning environment. For children who experience a lack of qualitative and quantitative language input at home, daycare centers might provide a valuable learning setting. Here, not only child-teacher but also peer interactions exert an influence on a child’s development (Wentzel, 2011). The peer-related makeup of daycare centers varies. For example, in Germany the proportion of children who do not predominantly speak German at home or who have a migration background averages 20% to 30% in daycare centers. However, the settings’ respective proportions vary from zero to more than 80%. Also, standard deviations of 20% to 30% illustrate that such differences are rather the rule than the exception (e.g. Anders et al., 2012). Local surveys show that such segregation patterns are not (only) due to country-specific or regional differences in the population structure but can also be observed within cities and city districts (Hogrebe, 2014).
Research on school context effects demonstrates that the social and ethnic composition of classes and schools exerts an independent influence on students’ cognitive skills even after controlling for individual characteristics and other institutional aspects (Thrupp, Lauder & Robinson, 2002). Mostly, these studies focus on the ethnic background or socioeconomic status as relevant contextual factors. More recently, Fantuzzo, LeBoeuf, and Rouse (2014) showed that student risk factors such as preterm birth, low birth weight or inadequate parental care are more important than student demographics relating to migration or income. Findings in early childhood education are scarce and inconsistent. Henry and Rickman (2007), Mashburn et al. (2009) as well as Justice et al. (2011) show a positive relationship between the peers’ average cognitive and linguistic skills and a child’s language competencies, but they only use information on four to eight peers and do not consider a center’s composition in total. Schechter and Bye (2007), de Haan et al. (2013) as well as Reid and Ready (2013) focus on the relevance of the socio-economic composition in early childhood institutions especially for children from families with low-income or a low socio-economic status. Research findings on differential effects are still inconsistent. Mashburn et al. (2009) find that children with better language skills profit from a beneficial peer context. In contrast, Justice et al. (2011) claim that better skilled children are rather resistant to context effects while less skilled children are positively influenced by a better peer-related linguistic environment. Also, children’s risk factors that have shown to be important in the school context have so far not been included in context studies in early childhood education.
Against this background, we investigate a) if the proportion of at-risk children in daycare settings affects the language skills of all children and, if so, which composition factors are decisive, and b) if differential effects can be observed for subgroups of children. The presented analyses are embedded in the project “Context effects of preschools: children's language skills at school entry” which is funded by the German Research Foundation.
Method
Expected Outcomes
References
Anders, Y., Roßbach, H.-G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., et al. (2012). Home and preschool learning environments and their relations to the development of early numeracy skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 231–244. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1990). The Ecology of Cognitive Development. Zeitschrift für Sozialisationsforschung und Erziehungssoziologie, 10(2), 101–114. De Haan, A., Elbers, E., Hoofs, H. & Leseman, P. (2013). Targeted versus Mixed Preschools and Kindergartens: Effects of Class Composition and Teacher-managed Activities on Disadvantaged Children’s Emergent Academic Skills. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 24(2), 177–194. Fantuzzo, J.W., LeBoeuf, W.A. & Rouse, H.L. (2014). An Investigation of the Relations between School Concentrations of Student Risk Factors and Student Educational Well-Being. Educational Researcher, 43(1), 25–36. Henry, G.T. & Rickman, D.K. (2007). Do Peers Influence Children’s Skill development in Preschool. Economics of Education Review, 26(1), 100–112. Hogrebe, N. (2014). Indicators for a Needs-based Resource Allocation in Early Childhood Education: Regional Data as Valid Proxies for Setting Level Needs? Journal for Educational Research Online, 6(2), 44–65. Justice, L.M., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C. & Mashburn A.J. (2011). Peer Effects in Preschool Classrooms: Is Children’s Language Growth Associated With Their Classmates‘ Skills? Child Development, 82(6), 1768–1777. Mashburn, A.J., Justice, L.M., Downer, J.T. & Pianta, R.C. (2009). Peer Effects on Children’s Language Achievement during Pre-Kindergarten. Child Development, 80(3), 686–702. Reid, J.L. & Ready, D.D. (2013). High-Quality Preschool: The Socioeconomic Composition of Preschool Classrooms and Children’s Learning. Early Education and Development, 24(8), 1082–1111. Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in Thinking. Cognitive Development in Social Context. NY: Cambridge Harvard University. Schechter, C. & Bye, B. (2007). Preliminary Evidence for the Impact of Mixed-income Preschools on Low-income Children’s Language Growth. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22(1), 137–146. Thrupp, M., Lauder, H. & Robinson, T. (2002). School Composition and Peer Effects. International Journal of Educational Research, 37(5), 483–504. Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Wentzel, K.R. (2011). Peers and Academic Functioning at School. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (eds.), Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups (pp. 531–547). NY: The Guilford Press.
Search the ECER Programme
- Search for keywords and phrases in "Text Search"
- Restrict in which part of the abstracts to search in "Where to search"
- Search for authors and in the respective field.
- For planning your conference attendance you may want to use the conference app, which will be issued some weeks before the conference
- If you are a session chair, best look up your chairing duties in the conference system (Conftool) or the app.