Session Information
99 ERC SES 04 E, Assessment, Evaluation, Testing and Measurement
Paper Session
Contribution
The potential of early parent-child interactions for children’s development is widely acknowledged (e. g. NICHD-ECCRN, 2003; Lehrl, Ebert, Blaurock, Rossbach, & Weinert, 2020). However, little is known about the internal structure and potential interrelations of different features of these interactions. Yet for child ages under three it has not been modeled as a learning environment of its own. In longitudinal educational research a general factor approach to parent-child interaction is commonly used in statistical analyses (e. g. Linberg, Lehrl, & Weinert, 2020; Nordahl, Owen, Ribeiro, & Zachrisson, 2020; Weinert, 2017). Nevertheless, differentiation, both in learning environments and in domains of development, prevails in older age groups from preschool onwards.
Drawing on the general “SSCO model of learning environments” (Bäumer et al., 2019) as well as approaches in other educational contexts like preschool, which confirm a three dimensional structure in group-settings (e. g. Bihler, Agache, Kohl, Willard, & Leyendecker, 2018), the differentiation of two dimensions of parental interaction behavior (cognitive stimulation and emotional support) is proposed for the dyadic setting. This model builds on, both theoretical (e. g. Bornstein, 2002) and empirical (e. g. Landry, Smith, Swank, & Guttentag, 2008) foundations. So far, only a few investigations have studied the factorial structure of early parent-child interactions (e. g. Linberg, 2018).
The following research question leads the investigation: “Is there empirical support for a differentiated structure of parental interaction behavior in the NEPS-SC1 observational data of parent-child interactions?”
Method
Using observational data from the first three waves of NEPS-SC1 (n = 739; Blossfeld, Roßbach, & von Maurice, 2011; Hachul et al., 2019) the proposition is evaluated through longitudinal structural equation modeling. Child age in the three waves was 7, 17, and 26 months, respectively. Parent-child interaction was based on semi-structured play situations of 5-10 minutes in the families' home, similar to the “three boxes procedure” in the North American benchmark study NICHD. Video material from non-participating observations was coded on a 5-point Likert-scale by extensively trained raters using a German adaptation of the macro-analytic NICHD rating instrument (Linberg et al., 2019). Interrater reliability exceeded 90% of agreement. This rating procedure provides both a holistic and economical measurement of qualitative aspects of parent-child interaction and thereby pedagogical process quality in the early home learning environment. Longitudinal cross-lagged structural equation modeling was applied using the software Stata® in order to foster deeper understanding of interrelations between the variables coded over multiple measurement points and give insight into latent structures. Missing data in the sample was handled through full information maximum likelihood estimation.
Expected Outcomes
Preliminary results favor a two-factor model of parental interactional behavior in the all three waves (Χ2 (38) = 186.92, p < .0001, CFI = .952, RMSEA = .073, AIC = 18614.807, BIC = 18854.283) over a general factor model (Χ2 (52) = 304.48, p < .0001, CFI = .924, RMSEA = .081, AIC = 18704.371, BIC = 18879.373). This gives reason to use a two-factor model of parental interaction behavior in the study of the early developmental contexts of infants and toddlers within the family, which allows for a more detailed analysis of this first learning environment. Further development, implications and potential difficulties of this approach are discussed. Differential modeling of parent-child interactions adds value to the assessment of specific effects of its dimensions on different domains of child development as well as, both the questions of timing and of potential interaction effects between behavior dimensions, under a longitudinal perspective (e. g. Vallotton, Mastergeorge, Foster, Decker, & Ayoub, 2017). As large-scale results in early childhood development mainly stem from North American studies, such as NICHD-SECCYD or ECLS-B, it is relevant to enrich the existing body of research with results from a comparable German study, and thus build a basis for comparative analyses like the one by Nordahl et al. (2020), which uses data from both the Norwegian BONDS sample and the NICHD.
References
Bäumer, T., Klieme, E., Kuger, S., Maaz, K., Roßbach, H.-G., Stecher, L., & Struck, O. (2019). Education Processes in Life-Course-Specific Learning Environments. In H.-P. Blossfeld & H. G. Rossbach (Eds.), Edition ZfE: volume 3. Education as a lifelong process: The German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) (Vol. 3, pp. 83–99). Wiesbaden, Germany: Springer VS. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-23162-0_5 Bihler, L.-M., Agache, A., Kohl, K., Willard, J. A., & Leyendecker, B. (2018). Factor Analysis of the Classroom Assessment Scoring System Replicates the Three Domain Structure and Reveals no Support for the Bifactor Model in German Preschools. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1232. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01232 Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Parenting Infants. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: v. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 3–43). Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum. Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Guttentag, C. (2008). A responsive parenting intervention: the optimal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal behaviors and child outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1335–1353. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013030 Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., Blaurock, S., Rossbach, H.-G., & Weinert, S. (2020). Long-term and domain-specific relations between the early years home learning environment and students’ academic outcomes in secondary school. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 31(1), 102–124. https://doi.org/10.1080/09243453.2019.1618346 Linberg, A. (2018). Interaktion zwischen Mutter und Kind: Dimensionen, Bedingungen und Effekte (1st ed.). Empirische Erziehungswissenschaft: Vol. 70. Münster: Waxmann. Linberg, A., Lehrl, S., & Weinert, S. (2020). The Early Years Home Learning Environment - Associations With Parent-Child-Course Attendance and Children's Vocabulary at Age 3. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01425 NICHD ECCRN (2003). Early child care and mother–child interaction from 36 months through first grade. Infant Behavior and Development, 26(3), 345–370. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(03)00035-3 Vallotton, C., Mastergeorge, A., Foster, T., Decker, K. B., & Ayoub, C. (2017). Parenting Supports for Early Vocabulary Development: Specific Effects of Sensitivity and Stimulation through Infancy. Infancy: the Official Journal of the International Society on Infant Studies, 22(1), 78–107. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12147 Weinert, S. (2017). The emergence of social disparities – Evidence on early mother–child interaction and infant development from the German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS). In H.-P. Blossfeld, N. Kulic, J. Skopek, & M. Triventi (Eds.), eduLIFE lifelong learning. Childcare, early education and social inequality: An international perspective (pp. 89–108). Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Pub. https://doi.org/10.4337/9781786432094.00014
Search the ECER Programme
- Search for keywords and phrases in "Text Search"
- Restrict in which part of the abstracts to search in "Where to search"
- Search for authors and in the respective field.
- For planning your conference attendance you may want to use the conference app, which will be issued some weeks before the conference
- If you are a session chair, best look up your chairing duties in the conference system (Conftool) or the app.