Following recent sociological diagnoses, it is continuing change (Rosa, 2014), global risks, and a reflexive interaction of uncertainties and opportunities (Beck, 2009; Giddens, 1994) that shape today’s societies. Some scholars suggest that people differ in their capabilities of responding to these challenges depending on their social background, for instance in their reflexive “identity capital” (Kraus, 2015) and their “reflexive habitus” (Archer, 2012).
Like under a burning glass, these societal diagnoses are even more visible in the enduring COVID-19 pandemic and seem to come with now unforeseeable implications not only for adults but also for children.
Importantly, a child encounters many ambiguous situations during childhood development in which conflicts arise between what the child knows about the world so far and what happens in reality (Brice, 1985). The child usually resolves these inner conflicts by changing their concepts of reality. Does this apply also to the present societal crisis?
Following Bronfenbrenner’s (1978) socio-ecological model, children as they grow up occupy one layer of social environment after the other on the micro level, starting with parents and the core family. The outer macro level includes “economic, social, educational, legal, and political systems” (ibid., p. 6f.) and frames an individual`s relations on other levels of the model. This is confirmed by results of a study with young people in the US and Europe, showing changes in 18- to 25-year-olds’ trust in public institutions and governments following the financial crisis (Schoon & Mortimer, 2017). However, do macro level changes equally affect all youth and children? Margaret Archer’s research on “reflexive habitus” (Archer, 2012; see also Sweetman, 2003) as an individual’s capabilities to deal with complexity and uncertainties, suggests that individual’s differ in these capabilities depending on their social background.
Besides children’s reflexive habitus, the type(s) of children’s trust—in how far they trust people in their direct social environment or people they have no personal relations with (van Hoorn, 2014; see also "trust in strangers" in Weigert, 2011)—might be another important resource of resilience in the pandemic. Importantly, social trust seems to be rather context specific (Möllering, 2006) and especially dynamic in adolescent age (Abdelzadeh & Lundberg, 2017).
Recent research on the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on children targeted for instance parenting styles and school children’s well-being (Neubauer, Schmidt, Kramer, & Schmiedek, 2021), the care work distribution between mothers and fathers of different socio-economic backgrounds (Möhring et al., 2020), and the perspective of parents on their children’s well-being during severe contact-distancing measures (Langmeyer, Guglhör-Rudan, Naab, Urlen, & Winklhofer, 2020).
However, research on youth’s and children’s own perspectives is scarce. The JuKo-studies 1 and 2 (data collection in May and November 2020 respectively) examined youth’s (minimum age: 15 years) experiences during the pandemic in Germany. While participants reported a feeling of disempowerment in JuKo 1 (Andresen et al., 2020), results showed a strong quest for political involvement into decisions in the consecutive JuKo 2 study (Andresen et al., 2020).
To the knowledge of the authors, the specific perspectives of younger children from different social backgrounds towards the pandemic remain unknown. This creates the relevant research gap for the present study as part of a cumulative dissertation, and leads to the research questions:
RQ 1: How do 7- to 9-year-old children describe their experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic?
RQ 2: What is the role of children’s particularized trust in persons, groups or institutions (micro level) during the pandemic?
RQ 3: Do children experience generalized trust into unknown people (“trust in strangers”) during the pandemic?
RQ 4: Do results of research question 1-3 indicate differences according to children’s social background?