Session Information
07 SES 02 B, Schools, Stereotypes and Tolerance.
Paper Session
Contribution
Are classmates’ evaluations of each others’ academic performances biased by a negative performance-related stereotype towards students with migration background (MB)? Will students’ evaluations vary depending on whether students compare themselves to fellow students who share or do not share a background of migration? As demonstrated by numerous studies in educational settings, classrooms produce a strong need in students to evaluate their performance (e.g., Levine, 1983). To our knowledge, however, no prior research on the evaluative consequences of social comparison in academic settings (e.g. Altermatt & Pomerantz, 2005; Lubbers, Kuyper, & van der Werf, 2009) has examined the impact of migration background as a social category on comparative performance evaluations among classmates. This seems somewhat surprising considering that at present Europe matches North America in its significance as a region of immigration.
Our considerations are based on previous findings of multiple lines of research. First, prior research in laboratory settings has found that evaluative outcomes of social comparison differ depending on whether participants engage in (artificially) created intergroup or intragroup comparisons (Brewer & Weber, 1994). Second, we know that potentially applicable and contextually salient stereotypic preconceptions can guide information processing by serving as heuristic cues that provide a quick basis for making a judgment or an evaluation (e.g., Macrae & Bodenhausen, 1998). Based on this, we hypothesized that students’ perception of de facto existing performance difference between self and chosen comparison partner will be shaped by a negative performance-related stereotype pertaining to immigrant students activated by intergroup comparison situations.
Method
Expected Outcomes
References
Altermatt, E. R., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2005). The implications of having high-achieving versus low-achieving friends: A longitudinal analysis. Social Development, 14, 61-81. Baumert, J., Bos, W., Klieme, E., Lehmann, R., Lehrke, M., Hosenfeld, I., Neubrand, J., & Waterman, R. (Eds.) (1999). Testaufgaben zu TIMSS/III: mathematisch-naturwissenschaftliche Grundbildung und voruniversitäre Mathematik und Physik der Abschlussklassen der Sekundarstufe II (Population 3) [TIMSS/III Items for assessment of basic mathematical and scientific knowledge and competencies of graduating classes in secondary education (population 3). Berlin: Max-Planck-Institut für Bildungsforschung. Bodenhausen, G. V., & Macrae, C. N. (1998). Stereotype activation and inhibition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Stereotype activation and inhibition: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 11, pp. 1-52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brewer, M. B., & Weber, J. G. (1994). Self-evaluation effects of interpersonal versus intergroup social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 268-275. Levine, J. M. (1983). Social comparison and education. In J. M. Levine & M. C. Wang (Eds.), Teacher and student perceptions: Implications for learning (pp. 29–55). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lubbers, M. J., Kuyper, H., van der Werf, M.P.C. (2009). Social comparison with friends versus non-friends. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 52-68.
Search the ECER Programme
- Search for keywords and phrases in "Text Search"
- Restrict in which part of the abstracts to search in "Where to search"
- Search for authors and in the respective field.
- For planning your conference attendance you may want to use the conference app, which will be issued some weeks before the conference
- If you are a session chair, best look up your chairing duties in the conference system (Conftool) or the app.