Session Information
24 SES 06, Influences on Choice and Performance
Paper Session
Contribution
Unlike boys, when girls perform a math test at school, they may be at risk of experiencing an additional pressure, namely the threat of confirming or being judged in terms of the negative stereotype of women’s poor abilities in math. For instance, the activation of such a negative stereotype causes a decrement in girls’ performance in math (Huguet & Régner, 2007).
This stereotype threat phenomenon (Steele & Aronson, 1995; Steele, 2007) has been generalized to many social groups (i.e., Afro-American, men, the elderly...) and to a wide variety of fields (i.e., academic skills, memory, sports...). It is mediated by situational reductions in working memory (Schmader, Johns, & Forbes, 2008). There has also been many attempts to understand how to reduce stereotype threat. One way to counteract stereotype threat effects is to reduce the negative propositional relation between the concept of group (i.e., women) and the concept of ability domain (i.e., mathematics, Schmader et al., 2008). This may be done by providing girls with a positive counterstereotypic model (i.e., a girl who succeeds in mathematics) when performing a math test. For instance, Marx and Roman (2002) showed that girls performed significantly better a math test when it was administered by a competent woman in mathematics rather than by a competent man (see also McGlone, Aronson, & Kobrynowicz, 2006; Marx & Goff, 2005). These results were generalized to role models other than the experimenter (McIntyre, Lord, Gresky, Ten Eyck, Frye, & Bond, 2005; McIntyre, Paulson, & Lord, 2003). Within the class, we identify two different types of role models for girls: a woman teacher who is competent in mathematics and a girl who is top of the class. The influence of the first kind of positive model has already been studied (see Marx & Roman, 2002), we focus particularly on the second. Thus, we hypothesize that girls do better in math in classes where the top of the class is a girl than in classes where the top of the class is a boy.
To test this hypothesis, we conducted a longitudinal study using questionnaires. A sample of 1082 students was followed during their middle high school years (49.8% were girls and 50.2% were boys). In the present study, we focus on the 7th grade, that is a sample of 811 students (392 girls and 405 boys) nested in 44 classes from 15 colleges. Among teachers surveyed, 35.71% (n =10) were men and 64.29% (n =18) women.
For these students, we collected their initial level of math performance (a common test they performed at the end of 6th grade) and their average score in math at the end of 7th grade. For each class we identified the teacher’s and the top of class’ gender (that was determined on the basis of the student whose mathematics average score at the end of the 7th grade year was the best).
Method
Expected Outcomes
References
Bressoux, P. (2008). Modélisation statistique appliquée aux sciences sociales. Bruxelles : De Boeck. Huguet, P., & Régner, I. (2007). Stereotype threat among schoolgirls in quasi-ordinary classroom circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 545-560. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.545 Marx, D. M., & Goff, P. A. (2005). Clearing the air: The effect of experimenter race on target’s test performance and subjective experience. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 645-657. doi:10.1348/014466604X17948 Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting women’s math test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1183-1193. doi:10.1177/01461672022812004 McGlone, M. S., Aronson, J., & Kobrynowicz, D. (2006). Stereotype threat and the gender gap in political knowledge. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 392-398. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00314.x McIntyre, R. B., Lord, C. G., Gresky, D. M., Ten Eyck, L. T., Frye, G. D. J., Bond, C. F. (2005). A social impact trend in the effect of role models on alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat. Current Research in Social Psychology, 10, 116-136. McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat through salience of group achievements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 83-90. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00513-9 Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. E. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336-356. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336 Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance? American Psychologist, 52, 613-629. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.52.6.613 Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797-811. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.797
Search the ECER Programme
- Search for keywords and phrases in "Text Search"
- Restrict in which part of the abstracts to search in "Where to search"
- Search for authors and in the respective field.
- For planning your conference attendance you may want to use the conference app, which will be issued some weeks before the conference
- If you are a session chair, best look up your chairing duties in the conference system (Conftool) or the app.