Session Information
09 SES 11 A, Assessing Linguistic Competencies: Phonological Ability, Spelling and Writing
Paper Session
Contribution
Research has offered various perspectives on how students learn to spell in English; however, evidence has not been substantive enough to provide consensus on whether spelling is acquired in progressive and distinct stages (Gentry, 2000; Invernizzi & Hayes, 2004), or in more complex, non-linear ways (Garcia, Abbott, & Berninger, 2010; Sharp, Sinatra, & Reynolds, 2008). Assumptions about the nature of spelling development have important implications for the way spelling is assessed and taught; however, assessment systems currently being applied by educators have failed to consider non-linear perspectives of spelling development.
Triple Word Form Theory (TWFT) offers a non-linear stance, contending that students are capable of concurrently drawing on phonological, orthographic, and morphological skills from the early years of learning to write (Berninger, Abbott, Nagy, & Carlisle, 2010; Richards, Aylward, Field, et al., 2006). As TWFT has been validated in a series of brain imaging studies (see for example, Berninger et al., 2010) and behavioral studies (Garcia et al., 2010; Nagy, Berninger, & Abbott, 2006), it offers an innovative and well-grounded framework from which to assess proficiency in spelling. Advocates of TWFT assert that efficiency and autonomy in the coordination of phonological, orthographic and morphological processing increases over time, and that developing proficiency is influenced by instructional priorities.
Although converging evidence offers unequivocal support for TWFT, stage theories are still widely accepted, and have been since their conception in the latter part of last century (see for example, Ehri, 1985). By analysing spelling errors that students make, stage theorists in the 1970s and 80s produced a linguistic index that subsequently led to the categorisation of spelling development into distinct and sequential stages (see for example, Bear & Templeton, 1998). It has been argued that existing measures of spelling achievement are “not sufficiently structured or standardised to provide the reliable, sensitive data that teachers need to plan instruction” (Al Otaiba & Hosp, 2010, p. 4). In most cases they merely provide a summary of words that are correct and those that are not. And yet, systematic analysis has the potential to identify breakdowns in phonological, orthographic and morphological processes, indicating that multiple linguistic components contribute to spelling (Silliman, Bahr, & Peters, 2006). Moreover, it is possible that different linguistic processes might be operative in different parts of the same word (Perfetti & Hart, 2002). It is critical that spelling assessment systems encapsulate the linguistic complexities associated with the particular language convention under investigation. TWFT provides impetus for an innovative spelling assessment tool to be developed and tested.
The purpose of the study discussed in this paper was to develop and test an assessment tool informed by TWFT: Components of Spelling Test (CoST). This paper presents the CoST as a new approach to spelling assessment, informed by TWFT but developed through a method of analysis suggested by stage theorists. Data used to develop and test the CoST were drawn from students in Year 3 and Year 5 across four schools in the Australian Capital Territory (ACT) (n=198). The results of the testing clearly demonstrated that TWFT offers a useful framework for the assessment of students’ knowledge in three overarching components of spelling. Indeed, the CoST provides teachers and educational researchers with a means to validly and reliably identify individual differences in specific phonological, orthographic and morphological skills associated with spelling.
Method
Expected Outcomes
References
Al Otaiba, S., & Hosp, J. (2010). Spell it out: The need for detailed spelling assessment to inform instruction. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 36(1), 3-6. Bear, D. R., & Templeton, S. (1998). Explorations in developmental spelling: Foundations for learning and teaching phonics, spelling and vocabulary. The Reading Teacher, 52(3), 222-242. Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, J. (2010). Growth in phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness in grades 1 to 6. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39(2), 141-163. doi: 10.1007/s10936-009-9130-6 Ehri, L. C. (1985). Learning to read and spell. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago. Garcia, N., Abbott, R., & Berninger, V. (2010). Predicting poor, average, and superior spellers in grades 1 to 6 from phonological, orthographic, and morphological, spelling, or reading composites. Written Language & Literacy, 13(1), 61-98. Gentry, J. R. (2000). A retrospective on invented spelling and a look forward. The Reading Teacher, 54(3), 318-332. Invernizzi, M., & Hayes, L. (2004). Developmental spelling research: A systematic imperative. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(2), 216-228. Johanson, & Brooks, G. (2010). Initial scale development: Sample size for pilot studies. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70(3), 394-400. doi: 10.1177/0013164409355692 Nagy, W., Berninger, V., & Abbott, R. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 134-147. Perfetti, C., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of Functional Literacy (pp. 189-213). Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Richards, T., Aylward, E., Berninger, V., Field, K., Grimme, A., Richards, A., & Nagy, W. (2006). Individual fMRI activation in orthographic mapping and morpheme mapping after orthographic or morphological spelling treatment in child dyslexics. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 19(1), 56-86. Richards, T., Aylward, E., Field, K., Grimme, A., Raskind, W., Richards, A., . . . Berninger, V. (2006). Converging evidence for triple word form theory in children with dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(1), 547-589. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn3001_3 Sharp, A. C., Sinatra, G. M., & Reynolds, R. E. (2008). The development of children's orthographic knowledge: A microgenetic perspective. Reading Research Quarterly, 43(3), 206-226. Silliman, E., Bahr, R., & Peters, M. (2006). Spelling patterns in preadolescents with atypical language skills: Phonological, morphological, and orthographic factors. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29(1), 93-123. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_6
Search the ECER Programme
- Search for keywords and phrases in "Text Search"
- Restrict in which part of the abstracts to search in "Where to search"
- Search for authors and in the respective field.
- For planning your conference attendance you may want to use the conference app, which will be issued some weeks before the conference
- If you are a session chair, best look up your chairing duties in the conference system (Conftool) or the app.